How strong is your mental imagery? It might depend on how 'excitable' your neurons are
Highly excitable brain neurons in the visual cortex may reduce a person鈥檚 ability to visualise things clearly, neuroscience study finds.聽
Highly excitable brain neurons in the visual cortex may reduce a person鈥檚 ability to visualise things clearly, neuroscience study finds.聽
Sherry Landow
News & Content Producer
(02) 9065 4039
s.landow@unsw.edu.au
The strength of a person鈥檚 mental imagery 鈥 their ability to picture something in their mind鈥檚 eye 鈥 is linked to the excitability of different brain regions, a study led by researchers at 麻豆社madou Sydney has found.聽
An excitable prefrontal cortex made a person more likely to visualise strong images, while the opposite was true in the visual cortex.
Brain excitability is the likelihood that neurons will fire, and it varies from person to person 鈥 for example, past studies have shown that people who experience migraines with aura have high visual cortex excitability.
鈥淪urprisingly, participants with less excitable visual cortex saw stronger mental images,鈥 says Dr Rebecca Keogh, postdoctoral fellow in the School of Psychology and lead author of the study. The findings were published today in the journal .
Neurons that fire more frequently in the visual cortex could be adding 鈥榥oise鈥 to the image signal, the researchers theorise 鈥 interfering with a person鈥檚 ability to form a clear image in their mind.聽
鈥淭hink of the brain鈥檚 visual cortex as a chalkboard,鈥 says Dr Keogh, who works with Professor Joel Pearson in 麻豆社madou鈥檚 Future Minds Lab, a centre that does both fundamental cognitive neuroscience research and applied work.聽
鈥淒rawing a picture on a dusty (more excitable) chalkboard would make it hard to see, but if you draw on a cleaner (less excitable) chalkboard, the picture will be clearer.鈥澛
The neuroscience study used a multi-method approach to identify the link between excitability and image strength, including analysing fMRI brain imaging data and magnetically inducing weak hallucinations (a method called Transcranial Magnetic Simulation or TMS).聽
After identifying a link between brain excitability and imagery strength, the researchers altered the excitability of a person鈥檚 visual cortex using a non-invasive brain stimulation (called Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation, or tDCS) to see whether it triggered a change to their imagery strength. Each phase of the research had 16-37 participants, with over 150 people in total.聽They plan on expanding this research in future studies.
鈥淭here are also large individual differences in our ability to create images in our mind,鈥 says Dr Keogh. 鈥淔or some people, the image is so clear it鈥檚 almost like seeing; for others, it鈥檚 weak and dim. Some people can鈥檛 see anything at all.
鈥淥ur research offers a potential neurological explanation as to why these large individuals differences occur.鈥
The cause of such differences has been a scientific mystery since Charles Darwin鈥檚 cousin, Francis Galton, discovered in 1883 that some people have strong imagery while others are born without any imagery at all.
鈥淕alton鈥檚 discovery was made in 1883, but some theorise the question could also date as far back as the philosopher Plato,鈥 says Prof Pearson, director of the Future Minds Lab.
鈥淚t is exciting to finally discover the first clues as to why the mental lives of each and every one of us differs so much.鈥
Dr Rebecca Keogh administering the Transcranial Magnetic Simulation (TMS). Image: Supplied.
To measure the vividness of a person鈥檚 mental imagery, the researchers applied a lab method that uses a visual illusion called 鈥榖inocular rivalry鈥 to directly measure the sensory strength of imagery. This method is more reliable and accurate than asking participants their opinion of how strong their imagery is.
鈥淧articipants were shown either the letter 鈥楻鈥 or 鈥楪鈥 at the beginning of each imagery experiment trial,鈥 says Dr Keogh. 鈥淭he letter represented the image they were meant to imagine: 鈥楻鈥 indicated a horizontal red pattern, while 鈥楪鈥 indicated a vertical green pattern.聽
鈥淭hey then had to imagine the red or green pattern for 6-7 seconds. Afterwards, a picture of the pattern was shown to them on a screen (the binocular rivalry display) for a brief 750 milliseconds.聽
鈥淲hen the picture disappeared, they reported which image had been dominant, i.e. whether they saw mostly green, red, or a mixture. We measured visual imagery strength as the percentage of trials in which the image they imagined was the image they saw in the binocular rivalry display.鈥
The stronger their mental image, the more likely it was to dominate the brief visual stimuli.
鈥淭his method bypasses the need to ask each participant their opinion about their own imagery, which we know is often biased,鈥 says Prof Pearson.
鈥淚nstead, the illusion seems to measure the sensory trace left behind by the mental image in the brain.鈥
The team also adjusted brain excitability through the non-invasive brain stimulation, tDCS. This procedure involved putting two small electrodes 鈥 one positive and one negative 鈥 on the sides of the head.聽
鈥淚n very basic terms, when you put the positive electrode (known as an 鈥榓node鈥) over a part of the brain, it can increase the likelihood that the neurons will fire. Similarly, if you put the negative one (the 鈥榗athode鈥) on the area underneath it, it becomes less excitable,鈥 says Dr Keogh.
This procedure doesn鈥檛 hurt 鈥 at most, participants would feel an itching or a tingling sensation on their skin.聽
鈥淢anipulating brain excitability levels caused the image strength to change, suggesting that the link isn鈥檛 just correlative, but causative,鈥 says Dr Keogh.
鈥淭his is an exciting development for using tDCS in potential imagery adjustment therapies.鈥
麻豆社madou Sydney's Future Minds Lab, which works to advance knowledge of the human mind, brain and behaviour. Image: Supplied.
Further research on tDCS 鈥 including how it works over longer periods of time and why some people seem to show larger or smaller excitability changes to electrical stimulation 鈥 is needed to assess how it could be used in potential therapies. If feasible, the therapy could help people with overactive or underactive imagery visualisations.聽
鈥淚n many mental disorders, imagery can become uncontrollable and traumatic,鈥 says Dr Keogh. 鈥淥ur data suggests a possible way to treat symptomatic visual mental images by non-intrusively manipulating brain excitability.鈥
Prof Pearson and Dr Keogh also want to see how their findings might explain 鈥 a condition where people can鈥檛 visualise anything at all.聽
鈥淭his discovery might also shed light on the timely topic of what causes aphantasia and hyperphantasia (highly active visualisations),鈥 says Prof Pearson. 鈥淲hile we didn't test either in this study, our findings hold the first clue for a brain mechanism that might drive these conditions.鈥
Dr Keogh says that any cognition that uses visual imagery will be likely affected by the strength of an individual鈥檚 imagery strength.
鈥淭hrough understanding what drives these individual differences at a neural level, we can potentially boost imagery strength and in turn, boost other cognitions that use visual imagery,鈥 she says.聽
Mental imagery plays an important role in everyday life and in mental processes. Whether remembering the past, reading books or in guided meditation, many people use visual imagery every day.聽
鈥淢ental imagery is a keystone mental process,鈥 says Prof Pearson. 鈥淚t holds the key to unlocking our understanding of how we think, feel, remember and make decisions."